Development of Language and Literacy

?
Strand and Hessle (2018)
Proficiency of English in EAL children improved with age.
1 of 185
Peal and Lambert (1962)
Bilinguals more advanced in school, increased mental flexibility.
2 of 185
Werker and Byers-Henlein (2008)
1. Sensitivity to phoneme contrasts for both languages remain. 2. Bilinguals take longer to attend to stimuli from native language, monolinguals take longer for unfamiliar language.
3 of 185
Ramirez et al. (2017)
English and English-Spanish bilinguals exposed to English or Spanish type sounds. No difference in reaction to English sound, but only bilinguals sensitive to Spanish sound.Frontal activation involved in EF.
4 of 185
Ollers and Eilers (2002)
Vocab smaller in each language but larger overall.
5 of 185
Bialystock and Feng (2011)
971 aged 5-9, monolinguals had higher vocab scores.
6 of 185
Calvo and Bialystock (2014)
Working and middle class bilingual 6yr olds worse than monolinguals on vocab.
7 of 185
Bialystock (2007)
The gap in number of words between bilinguals and monolinguals may persist.
8 of 185
Bialystock (2009)
Bilinguals show slow picture naming, poor verbal fluency, interference in lexical decision making and poor word identification.
9 of 185
Bialystock, Craik and Luk (2008)
Younger and older bilinguals scored lower than monolinguals on vocab and fluency tests.
10 of 185
Kaushanskaya and Marian (2009)
Bilinguals better at learning novel words that were phonologically unfamiliar. Not effected by seeing non-words that match English orthography.
11 of 185
Gollan et al. (2008)
Weaker Link Hypothesis: less practice using each language, link between semantics and phonology are not as strong.
12 of 185
Davidson and Tell (2005)
3 and 6 year old bilinguals use whole object constraint, older bilinguals resort to mutual exclusivity bias.
13 of 185
Kaushanskaya, Gross and Buac (2014)
Bilinguals better at learning a novel word when shown a familiar referent compared to monolinguals. No difference for unfamiliar referent.
14 of 185
Johnson and Newport (1989)
46 Chinese/Korean p's moved to US aged 3-39. The more exposure they had to English, the better they performed on a grammar test.
15 of 185
Fiege, Yeni-Komshia and Lui (1999)
240 Korean speakers, US immigrants living in US for at least 8 years. Later moved to US, rater as having a stronger foreign accent.
16 of 185
Hakuta, Bialystock and Wiley (2003)
US census 1990, self report measure, rated own English as poorer the later they moved.
17 of 185
Miyake et al. (2000)
Components of EF: inhibition, shifting and updating in WM.
18 of 185
Cromdal (1999)
Bilinguals do better at metalinguistic task requiring EF. Better at accepting sentence that is semantically anomalous but grammatically correct.
19 of 185
Bialystock and Martin (2004)
Dimensional change cart sorting task. Typical 5year olds stick with 1st criterion to sort, bilinguals find it easier to switch criteria.
20 of 185
Carlson and Meltzoff (2008)
Bilinguals better at conflict task, no different on delay task testing supression of motor response.
21 of 185
Feng, Diamond and Bialystock (2007)
Frog jump matrix, bilinguals only have advantage when extra rule is added.
22 of 185
Paap and Greenberg (2013)
No evidence of bilingual advantage on Simon, Flanker or switching tasks.
23 of 185
Paap, Johnson and Sawl (2014)
Studies showing bilingual advantage have small sample size, haven't controlled demographics. 80% fail to find advantage and few have been replicated.
24 of 185
DeBruin et al. (2014)
Publication bias, more supporting articles have been published than challenging ones.
25 of 185
Bialystock, Craik and Freedman (2007)
Patients referred to memory clinic for dementia. Onset for monolinguals=71.4yrs, bilinguals=75.5yrs. Bilinguals had fewer years of education.
26 of 185
Valenzuela and Sachdev (2006)
Mentally stimulating leisure activities most robust brain-reserve measure.
27 of 185
Bialystock and Craik (2010)
Bilinguals have smaller vocab and slower lexical access but advantages in EF and deferred onset of dementia.
28 of 185
Paap and Greenberg (2013)
3 studies showed similar pattern of executive processing with one task showing a bilingual disadvantage.
29 of 185
Pringle Morgan (1986)
First article on Dyslexia, 14 year old boy who couldn't read but highly intelligent. Deficit storing visual impressions of words.
30 of 185
James Hinshelwood (1900)
Congenital word blindness
31 of 185
Samuel Orton (1925)
Dyslexia as perceptual disorder, 'strephosymbolia' - twisted symbols.
32 of 185
Stein et al.
RDK to measure magno (motion) and parvo (detail) function. Dyslexics showed poor motion detection but intact parvo function.
33 of 185
Stein (2001)
Magnocellular deficit not found in all Dyslexics, correlates with irregular word reading but not decoding.
34 of 185
Bouldoukian, Wilkins and Evans (2002)
Show benefits of colour overlays.
35 of 185
Lavino, Fletcher, Brietmeyer and Foorman (1999)
No benefit of colour overlays.
36 of 185
Valdois
p's report identities of as many letter as they can
37 of 185
Bosse et al. (2004)
Valdois measure is a good predictor of reading scores in French and English developmental Dyslexics.
38 of 185
Ziegler et al. (2010)
Added symbols to Valdois measure, Dyslexics showed deficits in letters but not symbols.
39 of 185
Franceschini et al. (2013)
Action video games improved attention.
40 of 185
Gori et al. (2016)
Quicker nonsense word reading following action video games.
41 of 185
Nicholson, Fawcett and Dean (2011)
Automatisation/cerebellar deficit
42 of 185
Nicholson and Fawcett (1990)
Dyslexics have motor deficits.
43 of 185
Rochelle and Talcott (2006)
Motor deficit found in Dyslexics with ADHD, not a learning difficulty problem.
44 of 185
Raynolds, Nicholson and Hambly (2003)
18 children who had DORE intervention showed gains but not in reading or spelling.
45 of 185
Snowling and Melby-Lervag (2016)
45% prevalence, affected FR show sever language difficulties, unaffected FA still show poor language skills.
46 of 185
Hulme et al. (2015)
Language at 3.5 years predicts grapheme-phoneme knowledge and PA.
47 of 185
Hulme et al. (2012)
PA and LSK intervention mediated gains in reading in children with poor language skills.
48 of 185
Hatcher, Hulme and Ellis (1994)
Reading with phonology showed more improvements in 7 year old poor readers.
49 of 185
Serniclaes et al (2004)
Dyslexics are extra sensitive to within category differences.
50 of 185
Noodenbros and Serniclaes (2015)
Dyslexics have shallower identification of letters.
51 of 185
Messaoud-Galusi, Hazan and Rosen (2011)
Performance on speech perception didn't correlate with phonology or decoding.
52 of 185
Tallal
Rapid auditory temporal processing deficit, Dyslexics need a longer interval between tones.
53 of 185
Halliday and Bishop (2006)
Frequency detection deficit, Dyslexics less sensitive to differences in tone frequency.
54 of 185
Huss et al. (2011)
Onset rise time, Dyslexics less sensitive to amplitude changes.
55 of 185
Guttorm et al (2005)
Different right hemisphere activation in Dyslexics.
56 of 185
Law et al. (2017)
Dyslexics show sensitivity to sharp changes in amplitude.
57 of 185
Pennington et al. (2017)
Twin study, not all with Dyalexia have phonological deficit, some just below cutoff.
58 of 185
Thompson et al.
3.5yrs: LSK and FR predictors. 4.5yrs: FR, PA, LSK and RAN predictors.
59 of 185
Sakisida et al. (2016)
282 French 8-13yrs, phonological deficit most common, visual deficit least common.
60 of 185
Olumide et al. (2013)
similar activity in V5/MT during vision motor processing task.
61 of 185
Paulesu et al. (2001)
Reduced activation in left inferior temporal region.
62 of 185
Silani et al. (2001)
Difference in grey and white matter in left inferior temporal cortex.
63 of 185
Hoeft et al (2010)
Right prefrontal activation for word rhyme task, right superior longitudinal fasciculus white matter predicted by reading growth.
64 of 185
Melby-Lurvag et al. (2012)
Poor PA in Dyslexia, smaller deficits in rime awareness and verbal STM
65 of 185
Ehri (1995)
Stages of reading: logographic, alphabetic then orthographic.
66 of 185
Grough and Tunmer (1986)
Reading comprehension involves decoding and language comprehension.
67 of 185
Scarborough (2001)
Reading rope, language comprehension and word recognition.
68 of 185
Caravolas et al. (2013)
1. Early reading development slower in English, longer to learn inconsistent mappings, requires PA and speed of accessing verbal info. 2. English, Spanish and Czech tested 6 times in 3 years, Spanish and Czech better readers.
69 of 185
Rose (2005)
Phonic work should be prime approach to learning to decode and encode.
70 of 185
Share (1995)
Children learn written form of word by decoding.
71 of 185
Share (1999)
8 year old Hebrew speaking children learnt novel words quickly by reading them in context.
72 of 185
Cunningham et al. (2002)
Replicated Share in English, ability to decode novel word correlated with its written form.
73 of 185
Ouellette (2010)
Spelling practice improves orthographic learning, drawing attention to letter and sound.
74 of 185
Share (1995)
Context used to determine the correct pronunciation.
75 of 185
Nation and Cocksey (2009)
Knowing the spoken form of the word is the best predictor of reading
76 of 185
Ricketts et al. (2016)
Knowing the meaning of a word is the best predictor of reading.
77 of 185
Nash et al.
Knowing both spoken form and meaning predictors of reading.
78 of 185
Mackay (2008)
Adults better at reading aloud novel irregular words following phonological and semantic pre-training.
79 of 185
Wang et a. (2011)
Children better reading aloud irregular words following phonological and semantic pre-training.
80 of 185
Perfetti (2008)
Lexical legacy hypothesis, encountering in different environments makes high quality representation of words freeing up resources for comprehension.
81 of 185
Castles, Rastle and Nation (2018)
Reading comprehension requires multitude of processes, depend on purpose for reading
82 of 185
Kintsch and Rawson (2005)
Construction-Integration model: macrostructure, microstructure and linguistics,
83 of 185
Kendou et al. (2014)
Higher level processes: inferencing, WM and inhibition, attention allocation.
84 of 185
Yuill and Oakhill (1991)
Better readers make more inferences.
85 of 185
Cain and Oakhill (1999)
Older group poor readers less skilled at making inferences than younger typical group.
86 of 185
Oakhill and Cain (2012)
Inference making predictor or later reading comprehension.
87 of 185
Just and Carpenter (1992)
WM capacity determines how much info can stay active during reading.
88 of 185
Engle and Conway (1998)
Keep info active in WM for making inferences and monitoring comprehension.
89 of 185
Cain (2006)
Children with poor inhibition skills, less able to inhibit info, told to ignore.
90 of 185
McInnes et al. (2003)
Poor attention, less able to monitor comprehension.
91 of 185
Long, Seely and Oppy (1997)
Poor attention, easily distracted by irrelevant detail
92 of 185
Cain and Oakhill (2007)
Poor attention, difficulty detecting breaks in coherence in text.
93 of 185
Perfetti, Landi and Oakhill (2005)
Standards of Coherence, extent to which you want to understand and learn from a text.
94 of 185
Duff et al. (2015)
Measure language at 7years, look at vocab at 18months, no difference between late talkers and TD.
95 of 185
UK DLD Prevalence
LD=9.92%, DLD=7.58%
96 of 185
Bishop
SLI at 4 - 53% had it at 5.5yrs - 56% had it at 8yrs - 35% relapsed
97 of 185
Snowling et al. (2015)
Persistent DLD=56%, Resolving DLD=16%, Emerging DLD=28%
98 of 185
Gopnik & Crago (1991)
SLI family members problems generating plural, correcting ungrammatical sentences, producing derivations and inflecting verbs.
99 of 185
Rice and Wexler (2000)
TD Optional Infinite, omitting markers for finite verbs, DLD Extended Optional Infinite
100 of 185
Rice et al. (1995)
Past tense task, TD 5yr=92%, TD 3yr=50%, DLD 5yr=27%
101 of 185
Van de Lely (2005)
CGC, grammar deficit, problems with language that is structurally complex and hierarchically organised.
102 of 185
Miller et al. (2001)
Longer RTs on linguistic and non-linguistic tasks
103 of 185
Haylou-Thomas et al. (2004)
Simulated SLI by speeding up speech and increasing memory load.
104 of 185
Strong et al. (2011)
Repeat 2 tones, SLI impaired when tones short or brief gap in between. Findings not replicated.
105 of 185
Gathercole and Baddeley (1990)
Difficulty holding info in phonological memory, poor non-word repetition
106 of 185
Ullman and Pierpoint (2005)
Abnormal frontal/basal ganglia, deficit in sequence learning (procedural), grammar impairment.
107 of 185
Lum et al. (2011)
Verbal declarative memory impaired, both verbal and visuo-spatial procedural memory impaired.
108 of 185
Leonard (1989,98)
SLI result of processing difficulties, problem acquiring less salient features ('ed')
109 of 185
Joanisse and Seidenberg (2003)
Connectionism, anaphoric reference. Speech perception deficit - impaired PA - WM limitation - poor grammar
110 of 185
Badcock (2011)
Evidence for different functioning in DLD during language task.
111 of 185
Whitehouse and Bishop (2008)
Abnormal lateralisation during language tasks.
112 of 185
Graham and Fisher (2013)
FOXP2 mutation in family, Chromosome 7 and 16 associated with non-word repetition.
113 of 185
Bishop (2013)
SLI used inflected irregular verbs same rate as TD, use less overgeneralisations, more likely to accept incorrect grammar.
114 of 185
Lum (2012)
SLI impaired procedural visuo-spatial task, normal declarative, impaired, verbal STM and WM.Grammar correlated with procedural in TD but declarative in SLI.
115 of 185
Brown (1973)
Number of morphemes used increased with age as infants use more grammar.
116 of 185
Bohannon, McWhinney and Snow (1990)
Feedback is inconsistent and depends on grammaticality of utterances.
117 of 185
Saxton (1997)
Occasional contrast may enable developmental change.
118 of 185
Farrar (1992)
Infants repeat adult expansions of their utterances, mean they are salient.
119 of 185
Brown (1973)
U-shaped curve for acquisition of irregular past-tense/plurals
120 of 185
Messer (2000)
Early multi-word utterances reflect those most frequently exposed to
121 of 185
Lieven et al. (1997)
1-3 year olds used verbs in one type of construction
122 of 185
Tomasello (2000)
Syntactic knowledge may arise without semantic knowledge.
123 of 185
Chomsky (1965)
Can't acquire grammar from the environment due to inaccurate input.
124 of 185
Christophe (2003)
6-12wk olds can discriminate between language of different head direction, English head initial, Japanese head final.
125 of 185
Bickerton (1981,84)
Innate drive to learn language, Pidgins (simplified) and Creoles (syntactically rich)
126 of 185
Golden-Meadow et al. (1995)
Deaf children not exposed to sign language develop gesture system with syntax.
127 of 185
Pinker (1984, 89)
Innate knowledge of syntax and thematic roles, semantic bootstrapping, make inference about deep structure meaning based on surface meaning.
128 of 185
Gleitman (1990)
Semantic bootstrapping requires exposure to utterances with clear action and agents, unlikely.
129 of 185
Bowerman (1990)
Verbs that are easily mapped should be learned earlier through semantic bootstrapping but all verbs learnt at similar rate.
130 of 185
Schlesinger (1988)
Semantic assimilation, early semantic and syntactic categories , use action-agent schema to analyse new sequence.
131 of 185
Clahsen (1992)
Continuity hypothesis: all parameters available at birth but use is restricted by other functions
132 of 185
Felix (1992)
Maturation hypothesis: parameters emerge over time as we mature.
133 of 185
Snow (1972,94)
CDS challenges the claim that input is degenerate.
134 of 185
Dockrell and Messer (1999)
CDS: more pauses, shorter utterances, more redundancy, segmented
135 of 185
Fernald (1994)
Infants prefer CDS.
136 of 185
Lieven (1994)
Cross cultural variation in CDS.
137 of 185
Ellis and Wells (1980)
Rate of development not correlated with input.
138 of 185
Harris and Coltheart (1986)
CDS function not linguistic, may be to bond.
139 of 185
Pine (1994)
CDS may not necessary, but facilitatory.
140 of 185
Elman (1993)
Connectionist model, network able to learn grammar but requires feedback which infants do not receive.
141 of 185
Bates and Goodman (1997,99)
Vocab at 20months predicts grammar at 28months, no difference between early/late talkers.
142 of 185
Plunkett and Wood (2003)
Word learning is a relatively simple affair
143 of 185
Dockrell, Braisby and Best (2007)
Word learning requires extended, incremental learning.
144 of 185
Duff et al. (2015)
Vocab knowledge predictor of later outcomes.
145 of 185
Early Language Phases
Pre-linguistic 9months - Holophrastic - Telegraphic 18-24 months - Stage 2 Grammar 2-3 years - Adult-like Speech 5-6 years.
146 of 185
Brown and Hanlon (1970)
Adult-directed speech, encouraging poor grammar and using child-like words.
147 of 185
DeVilliers and DeVilliers (1979)
Breaking down words doesn't help if the child isn't ready.
148 of 185
Tincoff and Jusczyz (1999)
6 month olds can understand mommy and daddy but looking a video for longer.
149 of 185
Menyuk et al. (1995)
Comprehend 50 words at 13 months, produce 50 words at 18 months.
150 of 185
Golinkoff and Hirsh-Pasek (1995)
17 month olds look at correct picture, at single word stage but can understand word order.
151 of 185
Bates and Goodman (1997)
Relationship between grammar and production of 2 word utterances.
152 of 185
Ganger and Brent (2004)
Not all children show a vocab spurt.
153 of 185
Nelson (1973)
50 words: 66% object, 13% action, 9% state, 8% social and 4% function.
154 of 185
Carey (1978)
14,000 by 6 years.
155 of 185
Waxman and Senghas (1992)
Whole object constraint
156 of 185
Ninio (1980)
Adults make it clear when referring to whole object
157 of 185
Markmann (1992)
Mutual exclusivity
158 of 185
Landall, Smith and Jones (1988)
Shape bias
159 of 185
Markmann and Hutchinson (1984)
Taxonomic constraint: Similar category
160 of 185
Gleitman (1990)
Syntactic bootstrapping
161 of 185
Gathercole (1994)
Children happy to have more than one label
162 of 185
Smith (2001)
Attentional Learning: perceptual cues co-occur in a predictable way
163 of 185
Carey and Bartlett (1978)
Fast mapping, novel word matched to concept
164 of 185
Dollaghan (1985)
Novel word quickly associated by 2 year olds, remember word later on
165 of 185
Akhtar and Mantague (1999)
Novel word quickly associated by 15-18 months olds
166 of 185
Horst and Samuelson (2008)
24 month olds good initial learning but not 5 minutes later
167 of 185
Swingley (2010)
Establishing full lexical representation takes time.
168 of 185
Henderson et al. (2012)
Sleep is crucial for integrating novel words.
169 of 185
Gathercole (1995)
Speed of recall at 4 years correlated with current vocab and vocab growth.
170 of 185
Dumay and Gaskell (2007)
Lexical competition of new word only present after sleep. Podgy/podcast.
171 of 185
Davis and Gaskell (2009)
Hippocampus = plasticity, Neocortex = stability, integration of the 2 systems allow for offline transfer
172 of 185
Tomasello (2000)
Verb island hypothesis: each verb forms its own structure, age 3-4 become more productive with novel verbs.
173 of 185
Aitchison (1989)
The articulate mammal, similar features in animal and human communication, creativity, arbitrariness, semanticity, displacement, turn-taking
174 of 185
Jusczyz and Aslin (1995)
Recognise familiar words at 7 months.
175 of 185
Housten et al. (1997)
Remember familiar words at 7 months.
176 of 185
Liberman et al. (1975)
Synthetic speech continuum, place of articulation and voice onset time
177 of 185
Elmas et al. (1971)
Similar learning curve in 1 month olds, ba/pa
178 of 185
Werker (1973)
Sensitivity to language boundaries lost after 10 months.
179 of 185
Kuhl et al. (2006)
ra/la contrast, 6-8 months English and Japanese both sensitive, 10-12 months only English
180 of 185
Kuhl and Miller (1978)
Chinchillas able to detect phoneme boundaries.
181 of 185
Mehler et al. (1988)
Non-nutritive sucking 4 day olds, French infants show dishabituation when switch French to Russian, English only sensitive to switch from English to Italian
182 of 185
Bertoncini and Mehler (1981)
Discriminate pat/tap not tsp/pst, understand permissible syllable
183 of 185
DeCasper and Spence (1986)
Pregnant women read 3 stories, 3 day olds **** when heard familiar story and mother's voice
184 of 185
DeCasper (1994)
Pregnant women read story 34-38 weeks, foetus heart rate decreased to familiar story.
185 of 185

Other cards in this set

Card 2

Front

Bilinguals more advanced in school, increased mental flexibility.

Back

Peal and Lambert (1962)

Card 3

Front

1. Sensitivity to phoneme contrasts for both languages remain. 2. Bilinguals take longer to attend to stimuli from native language, monolinguals take longer for unfamiliar language.

Back

Preview of the back of card 3

Card 4

Front

English and English-Spanish bilinguals exposed to English or Spanish type sounds. No difference in reaction to English sound, but only bilinguals sensitive to Spanish sound.Frontal activation involved in EF.

Back

Preview of the back of card 4

Card 5

Front

Vocab smaller in each language but larger overall.

Back

Preview of the back of card 5
View more cards

Comments

No comments have yet been made

Similar Psychology resources:

See all Psychology resources »See all Development of Language and Literacy resources »